- Zoology
- Daily Critter Facts
- For Teachers
- Study Guides
- Diseases & Parasites
- Contact
The profound and often controversial question of whether non-human animals can acquire language has long captivated scientists, philosophers, and the public alike. At the heart of this inquiry lies our closest living relatives: the primates. The quest to communicate with apes and monkeys is more than a mere scientific curiosity; it is a journey into the very definition of what it means to be human, challenging our assumptions about the uniqueness of our own cognitive abilities and our place within the animal kingdom. For decades, researchers have embarked on ambitious projects to bridge the interspecies divide, and the results have been as illuminating as they have been contentious, forcing us to reconsider the very nature of language itself.
To properly evaluate the linguistic capabilities of primates, one must first establish what constitutes “language.” In the human context, language is not merely a collection of words but a complex, structured system defined by several key properties. These include semantics, the use of symbols to represent objects and concepts; syntax, a set of rules for combining those symbols into meaningful sentences; displacement, the ability to refer to things that are not present in the immediate environment; and productivity, the capacity to generate an infinite number of novel expressions. It is against this rigorous benchmark that the communicative achievements of primates have been measured, sparking a persistent and often heated debate.
The earliest forays into this field were marked by ambitious, yet ultimately unsuccessful, attempts to teach primates to speak human languages. In the 1930s and 1940s, projects involving chimpanzees like Gua and Viki sought to raise them in human households and teach them to vocalize words. While the apes demonstrated remarkable intelligence and an ability to understand some human speech, their own vocal production was profoundly limited to just a few crudely articulated words. These early experiments revealed a crucial biological barrier: the anatomy of the primate vocal tract is fundamentally different from that of humans, making the physical production of human sounds an impossibility for them.
Recognizing the limitations of the vocal approach, a revolutionary shift occurred in the 1960s with the work of researchers Allen and Beatrix Gardner. They hypothesized that the failure was not in the apes’ brains but in their mouths. They turned instead to a visual-gestural modality: American Sign Language (ASL). Their subject, a young chimpanzee named Washoe, became the pioneer of a new era in interspecies communication research. Raised in an environment where she was constantly exposed to ASL, Washoe began to acquire signs and use them in ways that stunned the scientific community.
Washoe’s progress was groundbreaking. Over the course of the project, she learned over 150 signs and demonstrated an ability to use them for a wide range of purposes, from making requests for food and drink to identifying objects and expressing desires. Most remarkably, she appeared to exhibit a degree of productivity, combining signs in novel ways to describe new experiences. Her famous coining of the phrase “water bird” upon seeing a swan for the first time suggested a capacity to create new meaning from her existing vocabulary, a feat that hinted at a deeper cognitive process than simple imitation.
The success with Washoe inspired a wave of similar studies, most notably Francine Patterson’s work with a lowland gorilla named Koko. Over several decades, Patterson reported that Koko had amassed a vocabulary of over 1,000 signs and could understand approximately 2,000 spoken English words. The project documented Koko using signs to express complex emotions, refer to past and future events, and even engage in what appeared to be deception and humor. While the scientific rigor and interpretations of Patterson’s data have faced significant criticism, Koko’s story captured the public imagination and further blurred the perceived line between human and animal minds.
In parallel with the sign language projects, another group of researchers developed a different symbolic approach. At the Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Duane Rumbaugh and Sue Savage-Rumbaugh created an artificial language called Yerkish, which utilized a keyboard of abstract symbols, or lexigrams, to communicate with a chimpanzee named Lana. This method removed the potential ambiguity of interpreting gestures and provided a more quantifiable, computer-logged record of communication, demonstrating that apes could learn to use and combine abstract symbols according to a grammatical structure.
This research reached its zenith with a bonobo named Kanzi, whose linguistic achievements are arguably the most compelling to date. Unlike other primates who were explicitly taught through operant conditioning, Kanzi acquired his understanding of lexigrams spontaneously, simply by observing researchers attempting to teach his mother. Kanzi not only mastered the keyboard but also demonstrated a remarkable understanding of spoken English, correctly responding to novel sentences and commands he had never heard before. Crucially, he showed a sensitivity to word order, proving he understood the syntactic difference between “put the jelly in the milk” and “put the milk in the jelly.”
Despite these astonishing results, the field has been shadowed by persistent and powerful skepticism. Linguists, most notably Noam Chomsky, have long argued that language is a uniquely human faculty, hardwired into our brains, and that ape “language” is not language at all. This critique was most forcefully articulated by Herbert Terrace following his own study with a chimpanzee named Nim Chimpsky. Terrace concluded that Nim’s signing was merely a sophisticated form of imitation to elicit rewards and that he never truly grasped the conversational, turn-taking nature of genuine communication.
However, proponents of ape language research offer strong counterarguments to this critique. They point to numerous instances of spontaneous signing, where apes use signs without being prompted or cued by humans. They highlight the creation of novel sign combinations and the evidence of displacement, such as apes signing about past events. Perhaps most compelling is the evidence of cultural transmission, where Washoe taught her adopted son, Loulis, to sign without any human intervention, a behavior that is difficult to explain as mere conditioning.
Furthermore, studying primate communication in their natural habitats provides a crucial context for the laboratory findings. In the wild, primates utilize a complex array of vocalizations, gestures, and facial expressions to navigate their social worlds. The famous case of vervet monkeys, which use distinct alarm calls for different predators (leopards, eagles, and snakes), demonstrates that semantic communication—using specific sounds to refer to specific things—is a natural part of their repertoire. This suggests that the cognitive foundations for language are not an artifact of human training but are rooted in their evolutionary history.
The decades-long investigation into primate language has not yielded a simple “yes” or “no.” No primate has demonstrated the full, recursive complexity of human language. Yet, to dismiss their achievements as mere mimicry is to ignore a rich and compelling body of evidence. Apes like Washoe, Koko, and Kanzi have unequivocally shown that they can master key elements of language: they can learn and use symbols, demonstrate semantic understanding, and even grasp rudimentary syntax. They have forced us to move beyond a simplistic, all-or-nothing definition of language and to appreciate the vast and nuanced continuum of cognitive and communicative abilities that exist across the animal kingdom, reminding us that we are not as far removed from our primate cousins as we once thought.